Testing Orientation, Concentration, Knowledge, and Constructional Ability
– Neurologic Examination > Mental Status Examination > Chapter 8
– Testing Orientation, Concentration, Knowledge, and Constructional
Ability
is mainly to look for evidence of any diffuse or focal brain disorder
that can affect cognition.
concentration, or knowledge whenever there is any clinical suspicion
for or complaint of cognitive dysfunction. Testing constructional
ability (e.g., clock drawing) is helpful whenever a disorder of
cognition is suspected, but it is particularly useful if you suspect a
focal right hemisphere process. None of these mental status tests need
to be formally performed if there is no clinical suspicion or complaint
of an abnormality of mental status or right hemispheric function.
clearly localizable to a specific neuroanatomic area of the brain and
should be thought of as requiring the coordinated effort of multiple
(diffuse) regions of the cerebral hemispheres. Constructional ability,
however, resides primarily in the nondominant (usually right)
hemisphere.
-
To assess if the patient is oriented to
place, simply ask where he or she is. If the patient is able to respond
correctly in a general way (e.g., in a hospital or in a clinic), try to
see how specific his or her answer can be, such as which hospital room
number or which floor of the clinic. If he or she is unable to respond
to the general question correctly, ask an even more general question,
such as what city he or she is in. -
To assess if the patient is oriented to
time, simply ask what the date is (including the month, date, year, and
the day of the week). If you suspect
P.27
the
patient may have difficulty knowing the exact date, start the
questioning more generally by asking what year it is before asking more
specifically about the month, day of the week, and date.
a task that requires juggling of information in the patient’s mind,
and, therefore, these tasks need to be performed without the patient
using pencil or paper. In practice, concentration is often tested by
asking the patient to spell a word (such as “world”) backward, after
showing that he or she can correctly spell it forward. Another test of
concentration is the test of serial sevens.
To test serial sevens, ask the patient to “subtract seven from one
hundred and then keep going down by sevens,” with the patient reciting
the answers one by one aloud. Avoid the serial seven test if the
patient’s educational status suggests that the calculation involved
(which is not trivial if the patient has only a partial grammar school
education) would be a problem.
about personal information (e.g., “How many grandchildren do you
have?”) or about current events (e.g., “Who’s the President of the
United States?” or “What’s going on in the news these days?”). To save
time, it’s probably best to start with a more difficult question and
proceed to easier questions if the patient has problems with the harder
ones.
One useful test for constructional ability consists of asking the
patient to draw a clock. To do so, draw a circle on a piece of paper,
making sure that the circle is big enough (e.g., more than 2 in. in
diameter) that the patient can comfortably fill in the numbers, and
then ask the patient to “Draw the numbers of a clock in the circle.”
Once the patient has filled in the numbers, ask the patient to draw the
hands to make the clock read a particular time (e.g., “Draw the hands
of the clock to make the clock read 2:30.”).
asking the patient to copy a diagram showing two pentagons intersecting
at one corner (so that a four-sided figure is created by the
intersecting sides); this is the constructional task included within
the commonly performed Mini-Mental State Examination (see Chapter 5, Approach to the Mental Status Examination).
(including the name of the hospital or clinic and the floor of the
building) and time (including day of the week, date, month, and year).
“world” backward if they can correctly spell it forward. Assuming a
baseline ability to subtract appropriately, patients should be able to
perform serial sevens down to the 50s or 60s.
clock in the approximate correct places, should not ignore any side of
the clock, and should be able to place the clock hands in the
appropriate places. Patients should also be able to copy a figure, such
as two intersecting pentagons, keeping both figures five-sided, and
they should be able to correctly copy the intersection of the two
pentagons as a four-sided figure.
consistent with any abnormality of cognition. This is not localizable
to a specific hemispheric region and is most suggestive of diffuse or
multifocal brain dysfunction.
difficulties spelling the word “world” backward or errors performing
serial sevens (assuming the patient has the baseline ability to
subtract). Difficulties with concentration are not localizable to a
particular hemispheric region and can be seen due to any cause of
diffuse or multifocal brain dysfunction.
address or information about family members, is usually clearly
abnormal. Be careful, though, not to overinterpret gaps in knowledge of
current events or knowledge of public figures (such as the name of the
President or especially the Vice President), unless it is clear that
this information would have been known at baseline. Abnormalities of
knowledge should always be interpreted within the context of your
presumption of the patient’s baseline level of cognitive functioning
(based on the history you have obtained from the patient or family) and
should be considered indicative of a new abnormality only if worse than
baseline. Most abnormalities of knowledge are not localizable to a
particular hemispheric region and can be seen due to any cause of
diffuse or multifocal brain dysfunction. Recently acquired knowledge,
however, may be lost due to deficits in short-term memory, such as can
be due to bilateral temporal lobe dysfunction, as described in Chapter 7, Memory Testing.
-
Errors in clock drawing can include
incorrect placement of numbers, repeating numbers, and incorrect
placement of the clock hands. Errors in drawing the intersecting
pentagons include any errors in drawing the correct number of sides or
the appropriate intersection of the two figures. Although all these
errors in constructional ability suggest the possibility of focal
nondominant (usually right) parietal pathology, most of these errors
are nonspecific and can also be seen as a result of any diffuse or
multifocal hemispheric dysfunction. -
A more specific and localizing kind of
error can be seen when testing constructional ability. Patients who
consistently leave out the left side of the clock (i.e., drawing all of
the numbers on the right side) or ignore the left
P.29
side
of a figure when copying it have evidence for left-sided neglect that
is suggestive of a focal nondominant (right) parietal cortical process.
-
Assessment of other aspects of cognition,
such as abstract thinking or judgment, can sometimes be useful. These
tests may be helpful, for example, when a frontal lobe dementia is
suspected, because memory and other aspects of cognition that are
routinely tested may not be abnormal early in this condition.-
To test abstract thinking, ask the
patient to interpret a proverb (one you would assume the patient would
have heard), such as “A stitch in time saves nine.” Don’t look for
perfection; simply listen for significant abnormalities, such as
extreme concreteness or any unusual response. -
One way to test judgment is to ask what
the patient would do if he or she found a stamped, sealed, addressed
envelope on the ground. There is no single correct answer to this
scenario, but listen for the general appropriateness of the response.
-
-
All the tests of cognitive function described in this chapter (as well as the tests described in Chapter 7, Memory Testing) would be significantly affected if language function is abnormal (see Chapter 6,
Language Testing) and, therefore, are difficult to perform and
interpret in the presence of aphasias, especially Wernicke’s aphasia. -
As described in Chapter 7,
Memory Testing, knowledge for self is rarely lost, even in patients
with a severe lack of orientation to place and time, and it usually
does not need to be specifically asked when testing orientation. The
loss of personal identity in an awake, communicative (nonaphasic)
patient suggests a nonneurologic cause of that dysfunction.